Sentence stress (/ˈsen.təns stres/)
If you have not followed the guide to
word stress, now might be a good time.
To analyse sentence stress (and intonation) we need to speak of
tone units (or tone groups or intonation contours, depending whom
you read).
The tone or sense unit |
On this site, we'll use the term tone unit and, following Crystal, we'll divide it into a pre-head, a head, the nucleus and a tail. In a sentence such as I went to London last week, normally pronounced with no special emphasis, we have:
- the pre-head I (which is unstressed to the point of inaudibility)
- the head went to (with a weak unstressed pronunciation of to as /tə/)
- the nucleus, carrying the key information, LONdon
- a tail, last
week which is unstressed.
The reason that LONdon is spelled the way it is is to show what is called the tonic syllable which is the most prominent (i.e., longest, loudest and highest pitched) in the tone unit.
The utterance doesn't have to be pronounced that way. If it
is said in answer to the question
When did you go to London?
the tone units might change to move the nucleus to LAST week
because that is now the key information.
Tone units are discussed in slightly more detail in the guide to key concepts in intonation because it is in the study of intonation that they play a crucial role.
If you are not familiar with phonemic transcription, do not worry
now. In what follows, we will be using very little.
There is a
course in learning to transcribe English sounds phonemically
on this site.
Timing |
A distinction is often averred between languages which are
syllable timed and those, like English, which are stress timed.
There are two fundamental forms of stressing.
The following is the theory.
- Syllable timing
- In some languages, such as French, Italian, Spanish, Cantonese and
Mandarin, every syllable is perceived as taking up the same
amount of time. This is the so-called 'machine gun' sound of
these languages. So we get:
I ... went ... to ... Lon ... don ... with ... my ... bro ..ther - The length of time it takes to say a sentence in syllable-timed languages is dependent on the number of syllables it contains, therefore.
- Vowels in syllable timed languages maintain their essential identity (although they may be shortened in rapid speech).
- In some languages, such as French, Italian, Spanish, Cantonese and
Mandarin, every syllable is perceived as taking up the same
amount of time. This is the so-called 'machine gun' sound of
these languages. So we get:
- Stress timing
- In other languages, notably English, Dutch, Farsi, Russian,
many Arabic varieties and Scandinavian
languages, some syllables take longer to say than others and this
results in a reduction of the syllables in between. So we get
Iwentto ... L o n d'n ... withmy ... b r o the(r) - In stress-timed languages, by contrast, the length of time it takes to say a sentence depends on the number of stresses it contains, not the number of syllables.
- So that the stresses fall evenly, speakers are obliged to weaken and shorten the vowels in unstressed syllables and that leads to the reduction of to from /tuː/ (pronounced the same as two or too) to /tə/ or even /t/ and the reduction of all the other vowels in the unstressed syllables so it sounds like Iwentlondnwimbrotha. That could be transcribed as /ə wentə ˈlʌn.dən wɪ mə ˈbrʌð.ə/.
- The upshot is that vowels in unstressed syllables lose
their quality and, in particular, long vowels get shortened:
/iː/ (as in been) gets shortened to /ɪ/ (as in bin)
/uː/ (as in goose) gets shortened to /ʊ/ (as in put)
/ɜː/ (as in nurse) gets shortened to /ə/ (as in about) and so do other vowels
/ɑː/ (as in heart) gets shortened to /ɒ/ (as in hot)
and a number of consonants such as /t/ and /h/ may be omitted altogether. - Languages which exhibit the phenomenon of stress rather than syllable timing are known as isochronous.
- Click here for a list of commonly weakened words in connected speech. You will see that they are almost all grammar or function words rather than carrying meaning content.
- In other languages, notably English, Dutch, Farsi, Russian,
many Arabic varieties and Scandinavian
languages, some syllables take longer to say than others and this
results in a reduction of the syllables in between. So we get
However, even if this distinction exists, it is not an
either-or one. Languages will vary along a cline from
syllable- to stress-timing tendencies.
(There is, in fact, a third form of timing: Mora timing. In
Japanese, e.g., a vowel (V) takes the same time to utter as a consonant
(C) plus a vowel so V takes the same time as CV and CVV takes twice as
long as CV.)
Here's a rough guide to which languages tend to be more or less syllable- or stress-timed:
BROADLY STRESS-TIMED LANGUAGES |
BROADLY SYLLABLE-TIMED LANGUAGES |
ARABIC (with variations) CATALAN DUTCH ENGLISH FARSI GERMAN PORTUGUESE (EUROPEAN) RUSSIAN SCANDINAVIAN LANGUAGES |
CHINESE LANGUAGES (also tonal) FRENCH GREEK INDIAN LANGUAGES ITALIAN JAPANESE (also Mora timed) PORTUGUESE (BRAZILIAN) SPANISH SWAHILI THAI (also tonal) TURKISH VIETNAMESE (also tonal) WEST AFRICAN LANGUAGES |
In the real world, it has not been possible to measure an
appreciable difference in the length of syllables across languages
which would support the division above and it is suggested that the
differences are not to do with timing so much as to phenomena such
as vowel reduction which are the cause, not the effect, of perceived
differences in rhythm.
It has further been suggested that all languages exhibit
both types of stressing to various extents.
It has also been claimed that different dialects of the same
language will vary in this respect.
This is not to say that the concepts of timing and patterns of stress are useless in the classroom but they do need to be handled with care. Alerting learners to the features of connected speech in English, in particular, vowel reduction in unstressed syllables and the pitch movements which occur in stressed syllables, is important both for comprehension and production.
As was explained in the guide to word stress, three things change when we stress a syllable:
- Loudness: how much force is used when saying the syllable
- Pitch: stressed syllables are often pronounced in a higher tone
- Length: stressed syllables take longer to utter than unstressed ones
Focusing on new rather than given |
There is a strong tendency in English to present new information towards the end of any utterance, a phenomenon sometimes called end focus. Take for example:
- A: What did she buy?
- B: She bought a shirt.
- the pre-head She which is unstressed and possibly reduced to just a 'shi' sound (/ʃɪ/)
- the head bought a (with a weak unstressed pronunciation of a as /ə/)
- the nucleus, carrying the key information, SHIRT
However, if the response is:
- She bought a shirt that looks awful on her
Then we probably have two tone units:
- She bought a SHIRT (which retains the structure above with the nucleus on SHIRT)
- that looks AWful on her (which forms a new tone unit with the nucleus on AWful, has a pre-head, that, a head, looks and an unstressed tail, on her)
The reason is that it is clear to both speakers that they know
the person in question and very probably also know that she bought
something. The only new information is, therefore, presented at
the end of each tone unit and takes the tonic stress.
New information, not shared by both speakers determines
where the tonic stress will fall in each tone unit.
Showing stress and tone movement |
In the classroom, it is necessary to develop and use consistently
some means of showing where the stress falls and how the tone is
changing on the tonic syllable. Each time a model sentence is
presented, for whatever purpose, it is helpful to learners if there
is some graphical way of drawing their attention to the sentence
stress pattern.
In the guide to word stress, three obvious choices were presented:
The same can be done with sentence stress with the addition of an arrow to show the tone movement. Like this:
Special stress |
We do not, unless we are praying or talking to ourselves, speak in isolation. We continually get, and sometimes suffer from, feedback from whomever we are engaged with in conversation. Take this example:
- A: Have you seen Mary recently?
- B: Yes, I saw her yesterday.
- A: Yesterday? I thought she was in Cambridge.
- B: She was. So was I.
It's probable that the sentence stress will fall like this:
- A: Have you seen MAry recently?
- B: Yes, I saw her YESterday.
- A: YESterday? I thought she was in CAMbridge.
- B: She WAS. So was I.
Which obeys all the rules concerning the stressing of new information and its placement.
However, if the conversation takes a different turn, things may alter to allow for the other speaker's feedback:
- A: Have YOU seen Mary recently?
- B: No, sorry, I haven't but JOHN has.
- John PARKer told me he HAsn't.
- Not THAT John. I mean the John who works in IT.
In this, the speakers apply special stress in order to respond to
the other speaker's emphasis, error or misunderstanding.
Everyone does that, in all languages, so it will not be mysterious
to your learners.
It is, however, important that we present the 'normal', i.e.,
canonical word stress to our learners before focusing on special
stress in sentences or we run the risk of confusing them. If
you do feel the need to practise special stress, it can be done by
taking a simple statement and varying the question. Like this:
- Statement: He spoke to the hearer at a party in London.
and so on.
Notice, however, that as the exercise goes on, the responses become
ever more unnatural because the word order does not change and the
tail of the tone unit gets ever longer. To comply with end
focus, most speakers will rephrase to get something like:
At the party in London he spoke to ME
or
At the party in London I was spoken to by HIM
etc.
Even with special stress, end focus will often rule the roost.
Special stress may also cancel out normal word stress so we might find something like:
- Is that your BiOLogy teacher?
- No. It's my GEology teacher!
Constituents |
There is a guide to constituents of phrases on this site. The following comes from the guide to post-modification of noun phrases where similar considerations apply.
We need to be slightly careful in deciding what exactly a
prepositional phrase is modifying or our hearers can misinterpret
what we mean.
For example, the sentence:
Jane spoke to the man behind the bar
can be understood in two ways, like this:
In the first sentence, the verb is being
post-modified and tells us where she spoke so the separated verb
phrase is:
spoke to ... behind the bar
Strictly speaking, we have a verb phrase and a prepositional phrase
modifier here rather than a pure verb phrase. A verb phrase,
technically, can only contain verb forms so the verb phrase is
spoke and all else is modification of it.
In the second sentence, the noun is being
post-modified and the object noun phrase is:
the man behind the bar
When the first sense is intended, speakers will insert a slight
pause between the man and behind the bar, making
two tone units each with a stressed syllable: the
man and behind the bar.
Disambiguation |
One obvious function of special stress and the deliberate moving of the tonic syllable is to disambiguate what might otherwise be unclear. For example, the sentence:
Anastasia hit the man with the chair
has two possible interpretations if no context or co-text is available:
- The man had a chair and Anastasia hit him
- What Anastasia hit the man with was a chair
The only way a hearer can figure out what is meant is by paying attention to how the utterance is stressed:
1 | Anastasia | hit the | MAN | with a chair |
pre-head | head | nucleus | tail | |
2 | Anastasia | hit the man with a | CHAIR | |
pre-head | head | nucleus | - |
Saying both of these aloud makes it clearer. Notice, too, how the timing nature of English forces you to compress the unstressed syllables.
Go to the index for the pronunciation section of the in-service guides |
References:
Crystal, D, 2008, A Dictionary of Linguistics and Phonetics
(6th edition), Oxford: Blackwell Publishing Ltd.
Kelly, G, 2000, How to Teach Pronunciation, Harlow: Longman