logo ELT Concourse teacher training
Concourse 2

Syntax: what it is and why we need to know about it

syntax

Many of the guides on this site are to do with syntax, i.e., the way words are combined in a language (English will be our focus) to make well-formed phrases, clauses and sentences.
Syntax has been studied in one way or another for thousands of years and there are active scholars today spending honourable and long careers studying just how syntax operates.  This is a short(ish) guide to the area for English language teachers and has no ambitions to be an original contribution to the topic.
The relationships to which the term syntax refers are syntagmatic and, before we begin, we need to discover just what that means.  Both terms derive from the Greek syntaxis which means a putting together or in order.


explain

An explanation of syntagmatic vs. paradigmatic relationships

Take the sentence:

  1. He bought a hat.

In this sentence, hat can be replaced by almost any noun but it must be a noun or a noun phrase.  Likewise, bought can be replaced by many verbs but they must be verbs or a verb phrase.  So we can get, e.g.:

  1. He sold a hat.
  2. He bought a car.
  3. He stole a gadget.
Syntagmatic relationship
This describes the relationship between, e.g., He, bought and a hat in Sentence 1, He, sold and a hat in Sentence 2, He, bought and a car in sentence3 and He and stole and a gadget in Sentence 4.
These relationships work horizontally between words.  Subjects use Verbs, Verbs sometimes take Objects, Adjectives modify Nouns, Adverbs modify Verbs and so on.  The relationship is to do with syntax (hence the name).
Paradigmatic relationships
These are exemplified by the changes we have made between the sentences and describe the relationships between:
    bought, sold and stole
    car, gadget and hat
These relationships work vertically in the sense that Noun phrases can be replaced by other Noun phrases, Verb phrases by other Verb phrases, Adjectives by other Adjectives, Adverbs by other Adverbs and so on.  The relationship is to do with word and phrase class.

It all works like this:

relationships

The words in each box have paradigmatic relationships to each other.  The red arrows show the syntagmatic relationships and it is those with which this guide is concerned.
It is simple to see that we can combine the seven words in each section in many thousands of ways, not all of which will make sense but all of which will grammatically flawless.


talk

Human language and the power of syntax

One of the defining characteristics of language rather than mere communication is that it is rule bound.  All human languages combine sounds into morphemes, morphemes into words, words into phrases and phrases into clauses according to abstract and often very complex rules.  To see what this means, take these five words:

  1. unicorn
  2. woman
  3. a
  4. the
  5. saw

There are in mathematical theory 120 ways in which these words can be combined (expressed as 5! for the mathematicians among you) yet, in English, only four of the possibilities are normally permitted with the remaining 116 forming combinations which are not allowed by the rules of English syntax.  (If we add one more word, the possible combinations grow to 720, adding a seventh results 5040 possibilities and an eighth brings the total up to 40,320 but of those only a very small proportion of combinations are allowed by the rules of English syntax.)
If you want to try combining the 5 words into well-formed sentences for yourself do so now.  Otherwise, click eye to reveal the four possibilities.

However, at the same time it does exactly the opposite: using the finite number of rules of syntax, we can create an infinite number of acceptable, grammatical sentences.  To demonstrate the power of syntax to create as well as restrict, consider the following possible sentences:
    The old woman in the red hat saw a contented unicorn in her garden yesterday
    My contented unicorn saw an old woman in a red hat in its garden last week
Now, it is a reasonable bet that neither of those sentences has ever been said or written before now.  Not rarely, never.
Nevertheless, the assumption here is that you will have no difficulty understanding the sentences (even if they seem unlikely to be true).  You can do that because you are able to apply the rules of syntax in English and extract the significance of what the string of words means.  What's more, you can do it yourself and create an infinite number of grammatically flawless sentences using the same syntactical rules we have used up to now.  It will not take much effort for you to produce sentences such as the ones above which are extremely unlikely to have been written or uttered by anyone else ever in the entire history of the human race.
All you need to do is apply the rules which we will now consider.


magnify

Discovering rules

While we know there are rules governing a language, it is less simple to discover and then explain what they are.
For our original four sentences above, however, we can hypothesise what the rules might be and then test the hypotheses and refine them if need be.  The process may work something like this:

  1. We know that:
        A unicorn saw the woman
    is a well-formed sentence (because our intuition as speakers of the language tells us so) and we know that:
        Unicorn a saw the woman
    is not acceptable.
    We also know that
        The unicorn saw a woman
    is acceptable but neither
        Unicorn the saw a woman
    nor
        Unicorn the saw woman a
    is acceptable.
    Therefore, our hypothesis is that articles must precede nouns (1).
  2. We can also hypothesise other rules because we know (again from intuition) that in:
        The woman saw a unicorn
    it is the woman who did the seeing and in
        The unicorn saw a woman
    it is the unicorn which did the seeing.
    We can hypothesise from this that the rule is subjects precede verbs (2).
  3. Finally, we can hypothesise from the four acceptable sentences that the objects of the seeing come after the verbs so the rule for that is objects follow verbs (3).

Now we can test our three hypotheses with some other data to see if they hold up.  We are fine with:
    A sparrow has eaten the seeds
    A storm had destroyed the crops
    The man owned a boat

and many thousands of other simple sentences.
So far all our hypotheses are supported by the evidence.
However, when we encounter:
    *A sparrow has nested the house
    *A storm had arrived the country
    *The man went the boat

our intuition is that something has gone astray with hypothesis 3 because none of those is acceptable.
We have to refine hypothesis three to objects follow transitive verbs.
Similarly, with:
    Has a sparrow eaten the seeds?
    Had the storm destroyed the crops?
    Did the man own a boat?

we run into a problem with hypothesis 2 because the subject is now sandwiched between verbs and we have to refine it to: subjects precede verbs in declarative sentences and questions follow different syntactical rules (which we can investigate another time).
Finally, we can try to find example in which hypothesis 1 (that articles precede nouns) is refuted and we find:
    The old man owned a fishing boat
and that single example is enough to cause us to refine hypothesis 1 to articles are the first element in noun phrases.

What we have done here is something like what many believe our students are doing when they encounter examples of language: forming and refining hypotheses as more data become available by a process of inductive reasoning.

Now we can take our rules and see how productive they can be.  Like this:

  1. Take a noun phrase and precede it with a determiner and an adjective so we can generate, e.g.:
        a tall person
        some old elephants
        few difficult problems

    and so on.
  2. Take a transitive verb such as:
        smoke
        truncate
        visualise

    and put that next.
  3. Take another noun phrase and precede it with a determiner and an adjective to generate, e.g.:
        the running vicar
        his beautiful cat
        that unpleasant rubbish

    and so on.

Now take one item from each of the three sections and combine them syntactically to make a clause and we might generate, for example:
    A tall person truncated his beautiful cat
or
    Some old elephants visualised that unpleasant rubbish
and so on.

Our troubles are not solved completely, of course, because there are rules concerning what sorts of determiners modify what sorts of nouns (so we can't have, e.g., several rubbish or a problems) but these are refinements we can apply once we have the general syntax right.
You will also have noticed that many of the possible clauses we can produce are simply impossible or obviously untrue.  That, however, has nothing to do with grammar because it's a lexical issue and that is one reason that we may refer not just to grammar but to lexicogrammar when we want to investigate the syntactical rules of English.  There is a guide to lexicogrammar linked from the syntax index on this site.


classify

Discovering categories

We have begged a question up to now and that is:
    Just how do we know that words and phrases belong to the categories we are dealing with?
We have encountered three categories so far: nouns (and noun phrases), verbs and articles (determiners of a particular kind).  We can extend that list to speak of adverbs, adjectives, prepositions, other types of determiner, pronouns and so on.
How we determine which item fits into which class is an essential step to forming the rules of English syntax (and the syntax of any other human language).
There are three ways we can do this:

maen

Meaning

We can start with what words mean and form our categories accordingly so we can state, for example:

  1. Words that refer to actions are verbs
  2. Words that refer to people or other entities are nouns
  3. Words that refer to the property of something are adjectives
  4. Words which refer to the frequency, time, place or manner of an action are adverbs

This will work as a definition for many words and from a set of rules like that it is possible to classify an alphabetic list like this:
dig, extremely, go, great, happily, house, Mary, often, potato, read, trivial, yellow
into a classified list like this:

Verbs: dig, go, read
Nouns: house, Mary, potato
Adjectives: great, trivial, yellow
Adverbs: extremely, happily, often

Unfortunately, however, it does not help us to classify words such as:
exist, loss, installation, openness
and thousands more because such words do not fit the meaning (or semantic) categories.  In that list we have:
exist which is a verb that does not represent an action
loss which is a noun that cannot be used to represent an entity
installation which is a noun that refers to an action
openness which is a noun that refers to a property of something or someone
and the list goes on.

form

Form

If meaning categories don't work, we can try categorising words by their form so we have lists such as:

  1. Nouns take plurals in -s or -es and can take a genitive ending in 's
  2. Adjectives take comparative forms in -er and -est
  3. Verbs take -s on the third person singular and -ed to signal a past tense
  4. Adverbs end in -ly and are formed from adjectives

and that will allow us to categorise, for example:
biggest, coldly, computer’s, corrected, crocodiles, cups, enjoys, exceptionally, fairest, fortunately, goes, hoped, longer, older, Peter's, wilfully
and then we get:

Verbs: corrected, enjoys, goes, hoped
Nouns: computer's, crocodiles, cups, Peter's
Adjectives: biggest, fairest, longer, older
Adverbs: coldly, exceptionally, fortunately, wilfully

But will not work with many words because:

  1. not all adjectives form comparative and superlative forms like this or at all (e.g., fascinating, absent, sunken, lost)
  2. some adjectives end in -ly (e.g., likely, motherly)
  3. some adverbs don't end in -ly (e.g., outside, often)
  4. lots of verbs are irregular (e.g., go, went, gone, put, put, put)
  5. some verbs take no -s for the third person singular (e.g., can, must, should)
  6. some nouns have irregular plurals or no plurals at all (e.g., information, sheep, mice)

Form is, in other words, an inconsistent way to categorise words.  It is not unusable, of course, because many words we encounter can be categorised by looking at the form.

cogs

Grammatical function

Here we are on safer ground and it is here that the concepts of syntax and word or phrase categorisation feed off one another.  We are considering the possible distribution of items within the syntax of a language.
The way to determine an item's function is to place it in a syntactical environment so, for example, if we are faced with the gaps in these sentences:

  1. She has lots of __________
    we can only insert a noun or noun phrase into the gap such a large dogs.
    We cannot, for example, insert in, happy, many, do and more.
  2. She can __________
    we can only insert a verb phrase and no other category such as dance well.  We cannot, for example, insert without, sad, because, radios and more.
  3. I saw the very __________ unicorn
    we can only insert an adjective or adjective phrase such as happy.  We cannot, for example, insert in front of, extremely, lots, smoke and more.
  4. She walked __________ the road
    we can only insert a preposition such as over.  We cannot, for example, insert because, yellow, and, carried and more.
  5. They have not __________ enjoyed the food
    we can only insert an adverb or adverb phrase such as greatly, often.  We cannot, for example, insert beforehand, delicious, some, read and more.
  6. She came __________ I asked her nicely
    we can only insert a conjunction such as because.  We cannot, for example, insert in, bicycle, however, do and more.
  7. I discovered that __________ student was late
    we can only insert a determiner such as a.  We cannot, for example, insert back, disconnect, although, perhaps, do and more.
  8. When I dropped __________, the glass broke
    we are constrained to insert a pronoun (and in this case, only one pronoun, it) although a noun phrase such as the thing is a possibility.  As nouns and pronouns perform identical syntactic functions, however, this is not damaging to the case.  We cannot insert, for example, happily, only, before, association and more.

This way of assigning words to word classes, while being the most difficult to apply, is the one that unequivocally works to categorise the lexemes of the language.  It is independent of meaning and form.


category

Alternative categorisations

In this guide and in most other guides on this site where word class is a relevant issue, the following categories of words and phrases are considered:

Category Word class Examples
Open-class content words Nouns house, place, happiness, Mary, Botswana, paper, luggage etc.
Verbs go, arrive, take, put, must, be, enjoy, like etc.
Adjectives pretty, helpful, blue, astonishing, alive, outside etc.
Adverbs happily, soon, frequently, greatly, noisily, so, very, accordingly etc.
Closed-class function words Conjunctions and, so, because, but, although, so that etc.
Pronouns he, she, it, mine, his, theirs, I, you, we etc.
Prepositions in, out, below, in front of, beside, between, underneath etc.
Determiners the, an, some, many, each, both, every, those, these etc.
The grey area Interjections whoa, wow, gosh, damn, ouch, my my etc.

Of these, the first four are considered open-class content word classes to which additions are readily made and which, standing alone, carry a signification which members of the speech community can readily explain.  These four categories also, it seems, are common to most if not all human languages.
The second four are considered members of closed-class, functional words to which additions are very rarely made and which, standing alone, defy clear definition and whose significance is only clear when they function in the syntax of the language.
The final class falls into a grey area because, although the words carry some kind of meaning in context, standing alone they do not and they also do not perform a distinct grammatical function.

There are problems with any categorisation of reality because items have an annoying tendency to slide between classes.  For example:

  1. The words this, that, these and those are determiners in:
        Take this glass
        I want that pen
        Try these glasses
        Pass me those bags

    but function very differently in:
        Try this
        Take that
        Give him these
        I want those

    where they are all pronouns.
    Other determiners such as one, each, all, both display the same slipperiness.
    (The word that also appears as a subordinating conjunction in, e.g.:
        She told me that was the case)
  2. The word outside is given as an example of an adjective which it is in:
        This is outside furniture
    but in
        She went outside
    it is an adverb
  3. The word since is
    a conjunction in:
        Since she wasn't invited to his party, she didn't invite him to hers
    a preposition in
        I haven't seen her since the meeting
    and an adverb in
        I saw her this morning but not since.

There are plenty of other examples of this categorical indeterminacy in the guide to gradience (new tab).

A further difficulty lies in the fact that although words may fall quite neatly into major classes, the function they perform, syntactically, grammatically and communicatively may be very variable.  There is no doubt, for example, that the word could should be categorised as a verb but it is one of a very different order from verbs such as cook and different again from a verb like be.

We should remember, therefore, that this is not by any means the only way to categorise the lexemes of the language.
Traditionally, what we have categorised above as determiners, for example, have previous been dealt with as separate categories for possessive adjectives: my, your, her etc.; demonstrative adjectives: this, that, these, those; quantifiers: many, a few, a good deal of, some etc.; articles: a, an, the, Ø (sometimes including some) and so on.
Quirk and Greenbaum (1973:18) also distinguish demonstratives and articles as separate word classes.

Other more recent categorisations, notably that of Huddleston et al (2002:22), follow something broadly similar to what is used here, although the preferred description is lexical category rather than word class, but with significant differences:

  1. The first four content-word categories are parallel.
  2. Pronouns are demoted from a separate category and subsumed as a subset of nouns (because that is how they act).
  3. The category of conjunctions is removed and replaced with two distinct classes:
    1. Subordinators: that, for, to, whether, if etc.
    2. Coordinators: and, or, but, nor etc.
  4. A new category, determinative, is proposed to include: the, this, that, a(n), some, all, every, each etc.
    This is to separate the grammatical function of determination from the lexical category of determinative.  So, for example, while the word that in:
        that car
    is a determinative, the same word functions differently in, e.g.:
        it was that close
    where it occurs as part of an adjective phrase.
    Determinatives considered in this way may only occur as part of a noun phrase whereas the function of determination can be achieved in many ways.

This may seem, and is, a somewhat minor and theoretical distinction but, because word class (or lexical category) is such a fundamental aspect of any understanding of syntax, it is, in reality, quite important.

We shall persist here and in other guides with the 9-part identification of word class that is set out above but that is more from considerations of usability in the classroom and familiarity than theoretical judgements concerning the sustainability of the concepts.

In the essential guide to word class, the relationships and examples are illustrated like this.  You can click on any of the green boxes to go to the essentials-only guide to that word class:

summary

You can get it as a PDF document by clicking here.

An alternative, simpler classification might look like this:
categories

and it would be possible to graft onto that diagram the various sub-categories of most of the classes.  We have not done so here in the interests of clarity but that is set out more comprehensively in the guide to word and phrase class.

It is worth noting at this point that the classifications used in this section refer to the English lexicon.  Other languages differ in having, for example, nominal classifiers as a discernible closed class of words (e.g., Chinese languages, Korean, Japanese and Thai) and in subdividing other classes (such as adjectives).  Many languages make no distinction between adjectives and adverbs and some rely on verb modifications and have few or no prepositions or even post-positions.
In some languages, such as Japanese, adjectives may inflect for tense and the borderline between them and verbs is somewhat blurred.
Even the distinction between closed- and open-class items is not a universal because, for example, in Japanese, adjectives and verbs are members of closed classes (with new items being formed in other ways) whereas pronouns are open class items (as they are in some other East Asian languages).
The only truly universal word classes seem to be those of nouns and verbs and words in other languages will be classified differently.

school

Spelling and grammar checkers

If you are reading this, it is a reasonable bet that you are also able to use a word processor to create texts of your own and part of your word processor package will normally be a spelling and grammar checker.  How does that work?

A spelling checker will, of course, be able to detect whether a set of characters between two spaces corresponds to any of the entries in its dictionary and it is possible, indeed routine, for users to add unusual or personally relevant items to that list.
For example, if we type shcool, the program can instantly recognise that this is a misspelling and will suggest the correct school as a replacement.  That works for words which only have a single spelling.
However, it will not be able to detect what is wrong in all these:
    That child has groan quickly
    We need to cheque the grammar
    I tried to catch it but mist
because there is no misspelling, just a misuse of homophones (groan/grown, cheque/check, mist/missed).  If you enter that text into most word-processing programs, nothing will alert you to the error.
Speakers of the language will, however, be able to recognise that groan is the base form of a verb which cannot occur after the auxiliary verb, has, in a present perfect tense form, cheque is a British English word for a money order and not a verb at all and mist is a noun describing thin fog.

A grammar checker uses the rules of syntax that it knows to flag up a possible word choice error by noting that groan is both a verb and a noun and cannot be used in that slot in the sentence as it stands.  However, mist is a noun and cannot be used in a verb slot but Microsoft Word's built-in grammar checker failed to spot the second two examples altogether, not understanding that mist and cheque are nouns that cannot be used in this kind of construction in that slot in the clause.
An example often produced by learners of an ill-formed sentence is:
    *She is know about the prices
and the grammar checker correctly identified it as a possible misuse of the verb know.  However, its suggestion for a correction is known which is also erroneous.  The program can detect a possible passive-voice clause, therefore, and note that the verb be is followed conventional by the participle -ed / -en form but cannot assign word or phrase class accurately.
As yet, none of the programs is close to being able consistently and accurately to check the syntax of English because:

  1. Words in a natural language may occur in more than one word class.  For example:
        The tickets are free
        She wants to free herself of any influence
        This is the land of the free
        The area is now disease-free

    In the first case, the word free is an adjective, in the second a verb, in the third a noun and in the last a combining form morpheme.  Any grammar checker has to include all four word classes in its dictionary to be able to decide whether the word is being used correctly.  Then it has to apply all the rules of English syntax (which are not yet known, incidentally) to decide if what is written is well formed or not.
  2. Words themselves have grammatical aspects which most programs will also miss so for sentences such as:
        The children has arrived
        The furniture are interesting

    no warning will be evinced even though the recognition of a plural noun vs. a singular noun should be easy enough to arrange.  It is, of course, but the recognition of concord and agreement between noun and verb is much harder.  If we take a sentence such as:
        The friend of my mother and my father are coming to the party
    vs.
        The friend of my mother and my father is coming to the party

    then an electronic grammar checker will be wholly at a loss to explain what's right and wrong because it is unclear to it how many people are involved.  Humans can recognise it instantly.

The grail of designers of such programs is to make them sophisticated enough to check such things and for that, they need a complete grammar checker and a grammar checker is just baby talk for a syntax checker.
Word class is fundamental to syntax and that is the reason that grammar checkers are, as yet, so inconsistent and frustrating.  Often, such programs will flag perfectly correct sentences as flawed (and applying any suggestions will usually make matters considerably and sometimes laughably worse) and yet ignore clearly flawed constructions altogether.
It's just hard.


truck

Using the categories

Once we have the categories of words and phrases in place, we can start to formulate our rules of syntax more precisely and, some claim, scientifically.  This means being able to construct paradigms from what we know are well formed sentences, inserting word class into the patterns we have perceived.  It works a bit like this:

Given a set of correctly formed sentences such as:
    The truck delivered some boxes
    The old truck delivered some boxes
    The truck delivered some large boxes
    The old truck delivered some large boxes
    That truck delivered those very large boxes

and so on, we can construct the pattern as:
S → Det (A) N V Det (A) N
in which:
    S = Sentence
    Det = Determiner
    A = Adjective
    V = Verb
    N = Noun
and in which the items (in brackets) are optional elements.  We can visualise it like this:

syntax

and from that simple diagram, we can generate thousands upon thousands of correctly formed sentences.  The number is not infinite but it approaches infinity especially if we are prepared to construct long adjective phrases such as large, blue, interesting, Chinese, diesel etc.
For example:
    My green dog ate her yellow cat
    Those large, vicious moles attacked that helpless, old man
and so on.
Furthermore, because syntax is, in theory at least, separate from semantics, it does not matter if the sentences make any kind of sense so:
    Someone's screaming concept devoured my unhappy car
is also derivable from the simple syntactical rule we have established.

We can do this kind of thing to form the basis of many different sentences so, for example, starting with
    Do you always have lunch in this room?
we can arrive at:

syntax 2

in which we have:
S → Aux + N + (Adv) + V + (N + P + Det + N)
in which the adverb is an optional part and (in the case of verbs which can be or are intransitive) so is the entire object noun phrase.  That pattern will generate:
    Can you sometimes see people in the street?
    Must he frequently drive buses on his holidays?

and lots more sentences, some of which may even make sense.


ingredients

Constituents

There are times, however, when generating the pattern is less straightforward because two possible patterns (and sometimes more than two) are possible.
For example, sentences such as:

  1. The neighbour of the woman and the man is complaining
  2. The neighbour of the woman and the man are complaining

have a single representation as:

syntax 3

but a glance at the two sentences tells us that the constituents of each part are not the same.
In sentence 1. only one person is complaining and he or she is the neighbour of the woman and the man.  In sentence 2., two people are complaining: the neighbour of the woman and the man.  The sentences can be written to show this by putting the constituents in square brackets, conventionally, like this:

  1. [The neighbour of the woman and the man] is complaining
  2. [The neighbour of the woman] and [the man] are complaining

Similar things frequently happen with prepositional phrases so, for example, in:
    He photographed the house by the river
we have two possible constituent analyses:

  1. [He] photographed [the house by the river]
  2. [He] photographed [the house] [by the river]

because in sentence 1., the prepositional phrase modifies the house and tells us where it was but in sentence 2. the phrase modifies the verb photographed and tells us where the photographing happened.

Word and phrase class analysis alone is, therefore not enough for us to form rules for acceptable sentences in a language.  We need to consider the phrase constituents of clauses and the elements of the phrases themselves as well.


identify

Identifying constituents

There are six main ways to identify the constituents of clauses and sentences.
We will use the same example sentence to show what they are:

He photographed the house by the river

  1. Making a cleft
    cleft
    There is a guide on this site linked from the syntax index to cleft sentences so this is not the place to explain what they are, how they are formed and why they are used.
    This is how they may identify the constituents of the example sentence, however:
    • It was the house that he photographed by the river
      identifies the house and by the river as separate constituents
    • It was by the river that he photographed the house
      identifies where the photographing occurred
    • It was the house by the river that he photographed
      identifies the house by the river as a single constituent.
  2. Questions
    ?
    When we pose questions concerning the content of a sentence, we do so by referring to the constituents so, for example:
    1. Where did he photograph the house?
      refers to the prepositional phrase as the modifier of the verb and identifies it as an independent constituent so the short answer would be
          by the river
    2. What did he photograph by the river?
      assumes the house is a separate constituent so the short answer would be
          the house
    3. Which house did he photograph?
      refers to the prepositional phrase modifying the house and identifies it as an independent constituent
          the house by the river
  3. Pro-form substitution
    bird
    We can use the trick of substituting a constituent with a pro-form to determine its role in the sentences, like this:
    • He photographed it by the river
      means that the house is a stand-alone constituent
    • He photographed it
      means the house by the river is the object
    • He photographed the house there
      establishes by the river as the separate constituent phrase
  4. Coordination
    juggler
    We can use a conjunction to coordinate a phrase to another and use that test to identify the constituents of the clause.  For example:
    • He photographed the house by the river and the boat
      establishes that the house by the river is a single constituent
    • He photographed the house by the river and from the bridge
      establishes that by the river modifies the verb phrase and that the house is an independent constituent
    • He photographed and later bought the house by the river
      establishes that the house by the river is a single constituent
  5. Making a passive
    cat
    Passive-voice clauses rely on raising the object of a verb to the subject slot in the clause (and we rename it as the patient with the erstwhile subject now the agent).  To identify our phrase constituents in the example sentence, therefore, we can produce either:
        The house by the river was photographed
    or
        The house was photographed by the river
    and in the first case, we know that the house by the river constitutes the patient of the clause and in the second that only the house is the patient.  It also establishes by the river as a modifier of the verb, not the noun.
  6. Mobility
    mobile
    We can determine what constitutes a phrase by the simple trick of seeing if we can move it and retain the sense of the original clause.  Grammatically, this is what we have done above to form passive-voice clauses and cleft sentences.  The simpler way is just to move an element, like this:
        By the river he photographed the house
    retains the sense of the prepositional phrase modifying the verb but not the sense of it modifying the noun.  However,
        The house by the river is what he photographed
    can only mean that the prepositional phrase modifies the noun and
        The house is what he photographed by the river
    retains the sense of the first example here.

There is a guide to clause constituents on this site, accessible from the syntax index, linked below.


structure

Phrase structure

Once we can define what constitutes a clause we can look a bit more closely at what constitutes a phrase and examine the rules of English syntax for making well-formed phrases which we can then combine into well-formed clauses and sentences.
To decide what the constituent parts of any type of phrase are, we need consider what is its Head.
Here is how we do that.

  1. Noun phrases
    things
    If we consider what we can insert in the gap in:
        __________ enjoyed the play
    it is immediately clear that it could be any of:
    he, she, that man, my friends, people, the woman in black, all of us, the people who waited in the rain for hours on end
    and so on, but the gap cannot be filled with:
    happily, green, shoot, up the road, has probably benefited
    and so on.
    This is because a noun phrase must contain a Head noun or pronoun (and for this analysis, following Huddleston et al, we'll combine those two classes as their grammatical functions are parallel).
    We can also see that a noun phrase can contain a number of optional elements.  Breaking down a noun phrase is written like this:
    NP → (Det*) (A*) N (PP*/S*)
    in which:
    NP = Noun phrase
    Det = Determiner
    A = Adjective
    N = Noun
    PP = a modifying Prepositional phrase
    S = a modifying Sentence
    * = may be repeated
    From that we can see that only the N is obligatory so all of the following will count as noun phrases:
        the man who came to the party with his sister (Det + N + S)
        she (N [subset Pronoun])
        the woman in the corner, by the fireplace in the red coat (Det + PP*)
        the nice, shiny, new, diesel, delivery truck (Det + A* + N)
    The rule we have will not generate:
    the happily man, go house, many pretty etc. because they break the rules for forming noun phrases and cannot fill the gap in the sentence.
  2. Verb phrases
    action
    By the same token, verb phrases must contain a Head verb but other elements are optional.  So, to fill the gap in:
        My mother __________
    we could have:
    wanted some cake, rang, has tried to get her money back, told me she was getting married again, arrived by car
    and so on but we cannot fill the gap with:
    happy, in London, the garden
    and so on.
    Thus a verb phrase is:
    VP → V (NP) (PP*/S*)
    in which only the V is obligatory.  The following do not qualify as verb phrases:
    very happy, the wide road, the building, under the carpet
    because they do not contain the obligatory V.
    We can also see, incidentally, that in order to form a correct sentence in English the VP is an obligatory element.
    From that we can see that only the V is obligatory so all of the following will count as verb phrases:
        left (V)
        left the house (V + NP)
        came to the party which was arranged by the company (V + PP+ S)
    However, verb phrases do not stop there because we can also fill the gap in our example sentence with:
    can play the piano, spoke too loudly, watched from the balcony
    and in these cases, we can further define a verb phrase as:
    VP → (Aux) V (NP)
    or
    VP → V (Adv/PP)
    where Adv = modifying adverbial.
    It is worth pausing here to consider finite and non-finite verb forms, both of which can appear in VPs.  A finite verb form is marked for tense or person (even when that marking is, as is often the case in English, zero or Ø) so a finite verb phrase might be:
        she works
    with the s ending on the verb marking it for person or:
        they work
    with a Ø inflexion on the verb.
    A non-finite verb phrase might be
        to open the window
    in which the verb is unmarked and a non-finite form.
    Some verbs, including the central modal auxiliaries, have no non-finite forms, incidentally.
  3. Adjective phrases
    alone
    must contain at least one adjective as the Head but may contain other elements.
    To fill the gap in:
        Mary seemed __________
    we can select elements such as:
    tired, happy to be there, aware of the problem, happy that her son was going to university
    and so on but not
    the house, enjoy, in the garden
    and so on.
    So an adjective phrase is:
    AP → A (PP/VP/S)
    denoting that these are adjective phrases:
    happy in herself, delighted that she could be there, pleased to help
    but not
    over the road, want to come
    because the Head is missing.
  4. Adverb phrases
    accurately
    must contain at least one adverb as the Head but may be quite complex.
    To fill the gap in:
        John spoke very __________
    we can select:
    loudly, frequently, privately, persuasively, almost endlessly, twice, again and again
    and so on but we cannot select:
    happy, in the garden, the house
    because the syntactical rules of English forbid this.
    Thus:
    AdvP → (AdvP) Adv
    Which willgenerate:
        movingly (Adv)
        lengthily at the meeting (Adv + AdvP)
    and so on.  In the second example, we have a prepositional phrase, of course, but, because it is adverbial in nature, we can represent it as AdvP.
  5. Prepositional phrases
    box
    are even simpler and we can fill the gap in:
        Mary arrived __________
    with an adverb phrase as we saw above but also with a prepositional phrase such as:
    at the hotel, before six, by car, with her mother
    and so on but not with
    the hotel, goes, who wanted to see her
    Thus:
    PP → P NP
    which means that both the P and the NP are obligatory constituents.
    (In some analyses, a preposition may stand alone and be designated as an intransitive preposition so, by that analysis:
        She went outside
    is represented as
        N + V + P
    but that is not the line taken here.  We would analyse that as:
        N + V + Adv
    with outside categorised as an adverb.)

If you like, you can take two little matching tests to see if you have understood this section.

An alternative classification of phrase classes is reasonably similar to what has been discussed here and, based on Huddleston et al (2002:23), looks like this:

phrases

Elsewhere on this site, this is not the categorisation which is generally followed and, although it is stated that a verb phrase may only contains verbs, it is conceded that, for teaching purposes, the verb plus any modifiers, the object and the adverbials may be considered together as a single phrase.

There is one other category in that classification that has not yet been explained: the nominal.
Most analyses will not see a nominal as a separate and distinct form of phrase but doing so has some utility.  Almost any phrase in English may be nominalised and therefore act as if it was a noun phrase in terms of syntax so, for example, in the following we have:
    In the corner is the best place for that
in which the prepositional phrase is nominalised.
    Only the young and fit are able to do that
in which we have a nominalised adjective phrase.
    Being criticised irritated her
in which we have a nominalised non-finite verb phrase.
    Enthusiastically is how they responded
in which the adverb is nominalised.
    That he was allowed to go home was a sensible decision
in which we have a nominalised subordinate clause.
    They got married which pleased her mother
in which we have a nominalised finite verb phrase (including the subject noun).
There are guides to nominalisation linked from the syntax index.


nuts

The nuts and bolts: putting it all together

We have now reached the stage where we can recap and see how it all works in combination.  So far, we have established that:

  1. A sentence in its minimal form consists of a noun phrase plus a verb phrase or:
    S → NP VP
    and that will generate, for example:
        They went home
    and verb phrases, as we saw have a number of optional potential elements (see 3., below).
  2. A noun phrase consists minimally of a noun but may have optional elements like this:
    NP → (Det*) (A*) N (PP*/S*)
    and that pattern will generate, for example:
        women
        the woman
        the young woman

        the old car in the garage with the broken windscreen
        his old computer I repaired last week
  3. A verb phrase consists minimally of a verb but may have optional elements like this:
    VP → V (NP) (PP*/S*)
    which might generate:
        has left the keys under the mat
        went to London along with his sister to see the changing of the guard
    or it may appear as:
    VP → (Aux) V (NP)
    which could generate:
        can cook dinner
        had a late lunch
    or
    VP → V (Adv/PP)
    which will generate, for example:
        go frequently to the shops
        lie in the sun
  4. An adjective phrase consists minimally of an adjective but may have optional elements like this:
    AP → (Adv) A (PP/VP/S)
    which could generate:
        very happy in my new home
        delighted you could make it
        probably happy to help
  5. An adverb phrase consists minimally of an adverb but may have an optional extra adverb phrase like this:
    AdvP → Adv (AdvP)
    which could generate:
        very quickly but very carefully
  6. A prepositional phrase must consist of the preposition plus a noun phrase, like this:
    PP → P NP
    and that could generate:
        into the old suitcase

If we add all these elements into the structure of a complete sentence we can generate something like:

The young man from London could buy the most expensive car in the showroom

which looks complicated but is actually not because we can represent it this way:

syntax 4

It is also possible to break down the adjective phrase (into Adv [most] and Adj [expensive]) and the final noun phrase (into N [car], P [in], Det [the] and N [showroom]) but that is not necessary once we know how to do it.

In theory, all sentences and clauses in any language can be generated by applying the rules for what forms a sentence and what forms the components of its constituent phrases.
However, what we have done here applies to English syntax and it is unlikely that other languages will work in the same way although the assumption made by many is that all languages contain the basic building blocks.

Now, armed with our ability to identify the functions of phrases within clauses and sentences, we can disambiguate our previous example:
    He photographed the house by the river
without recourse to square brackets and so on to say what the phrases are doing.  It will work like this:

Interpretation 1
[He] photographed [the house by the river]
Interpretation 2
[He] photographed [the house] [by the river]
syntax 7 syntax 8

and it is simple to see that in interpretation 1, the noun phrase includes the prepositional phrase but in interpretation 2, the verb phrase includes the prepositional phrase.


cogs

Recursion

One of the most important features of human language which distinguishes it from any other form of communication is recursiveness which means we can apply the same rules for forming acceptable syntax again and again.  We saw this in the last example in which a prepositional phrase was included within the final noun phrase.
Now we'll consider these sentence:

  1. John believes Mary is going to be angry
  2. I know John believes Mary is going to be angry

In the first sentence we have a simple structure like this:
S → N + VP
and we have the verb phrase like this:
VP → V + S
so we can represent that as:

syntax 5

Sentence 2. is formed by applying the same rule recursively and looks like this:

syntax 6

and we can, of course go on with the process without end (the sentence could be infinitely long) so we might have:
    Some people have guessed that you think I know John believes Mary is going to be angry
but we won't.
We can also break down the final S at the bottom right of the tree into:
mary
and that could be appended to our diagram quite easily but exactly how one analyses the verb phrase is open to some debate.  Some recognise be going to as a constituent auxiliary verb phrase and some would append the to to the verb be.  For teaching purposes, it matters little which approach you take, providing you are consistent.


teaching

In the classroom

The second part of the title of this guide is and why we need to know about it.
Here's why:

Guesswork
blind
We can, of course, try to teach the rules of English syntax purely inductively and hope that our learners are alert enough to be able to figure out three things for each type of phrase they encounter:
  1. What the obligatory Head is for each phrase type
  2. What the optional constituents are and whether they can be repeated
  3. What elements are forbidden by the rules of English syntax for each phrase type
However, it is somewhat unlikely that they will be able to do this in every case because, as we saw above, phrases may contain a range of optional and obligatory elements and, to make things worse, the elements they contain may vary from language to language.
We can help a little with the process by making sure that the phrases we present together contain the same elements in the same order so that it makes some kind of pattern sense and we can start with the easiest elements before adding in longer and more complex ones.
This means, for example, presenting phrases like this:
  1. NP: present as
    • N
      such as in:
      She came home
      Mary arrived

      etc. before
    • Det N
      such as in:
      The weather is terrible
      A man
      arrived

      etc. before
    • Det Adj N
      such as in:
      That old Ford car is cheap
      Those beautiful, fresh flowers are for her

      etc. before
    • Det Adj N PP
      such as in:
      The wonderful pictures on the walls in the gallery are very valuable
      Some interesting ideas from the meeting about the new road were suggested

      etc.
  2. VP: present as:
    • VP → V NP
      such as in:
      She read a book
      We enjoyed the meal

      etc. or
      VP → V Adv
      such as in:
      I walked slowly
      Please smoke outside
      etc. or
      VP → V PP
      such as in:
      She sat in the corner
      She went to school

      etc. before
    • VP → Aux V NP
      such as in:
      Mary has finished her breakfast
      We can answer the questions

      etc. before
    • VP → V NP PP
      such as in:
      I have put the old chairs in the corner
      Mary must arrive before six o'clock

      etc. before
    • VP → V NP S
      such as in:
      I'll buy the book John recommended to me
      Angela printed out the instructions we have followed to get here
      etc.
and so on for other phrase types, starting with the minimal obligatory elements (i.e., the Head of each phrase) and moving on to adding in optional constituents as learners become more confident and able to cope with additions to the structure.
Presentation
present
It makes sense when the focus in a lesson is on acceptable syntactic structures, to focus any presentation on just one or two so that the learners are able to notice the patterns or, indeed, have them pointed out.
For example, if noun phrases are presented with and without adjectives or modifying prepositional phrases, with and without suitable determiners and so on in the same lesson, it will not be surprising if our learners fail to notice any of the patterns we have analysed in this guide.
Equally, if verb phrase patterns are mixed to include those with and without adverb phrases, using transitive and intransitive verbs randomly (so with and without NP constituents) and some with prepositional phrase modifiers and others without, then the learners will not have the data from which to realise which patterns are acceptable and which forbidden.
Adjective phrases, too, despite being usually quite simple can contain large quantities of text functioning as a prepositional phrase, a verb phrase or a whole sentence.
These are wasted opportunities which could easily lead to errors such as:
    *She is know about the prices
rather than the intended:
    She is sure about the prices
because the learner has not been led to understand how adjective phrases function in English.
Unpacking complexity
unpack
A great strength of some syntactical analysis in classrooms is that it can empower learners armed with a little knowledge to unpack seemingly complex and otherwise impenetrable sentences because they are alert to the fundamental constituents of clauses and can strip out the optional elements to leave the core.
We can, for example, take something like:

The storm that swept in last night from the sea almost completely destroyed the villagers' fragile and unprotected fishing boats

and bring some phrase analysis to bear on it, stripping out the optional constituents of the two noun phrases and the verb phrase so we are left with:

The storm destroyed the boats

which is all that is required for adequate comprehension in most circumstances.
The ability to recognise clause elements and then to recognise the next level down of phrase elements is a skill well worth encouraging and nurturing in your learners because it can make them independent readers and listeners more quickly than any amount of skills work practice.
Correction
mark
When correcting spoken or written work, it pays dividends on many occasions to be alert to ill-formed or incomplete phrase constituents so, for example, errors such as:
    *He proud to be here
    *That in the corner was broken
    *My mother in the garden
    *In the house is decorated

and so on are all easily correctable, of course, but they will recur unless the learners can be brought to the understanding (by telling, by elicitation or by exemplification) that certain types of phrases require certain elements in English.  In these cases, the focus needs to be on the syntactical constituents of adjective phrases, noun phrases, verb phrases and noun phrases (again) respectively.

teaching

More on syntax

It is usual for guides on this site to end with a table of links to related guides.  That's not happening here because syntax is related to just about everything to do with grammar and language analysis so, for more in the area, go to the in-service syntax index and select where you want to go from there.


References:
Huddleston, R and Pullum, GK et al, 2002, The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Quirk, R and Greenbaum, S, 1973, A University Grammar of English, Harlow: Longman