logo ELT Concourse teacher training
Concourse 2

Krashen and the Natural Approach

deer

The guide to the history and development of English Language Teaching covers: grammar translation, audio-lingualism (including structural linguistics and behaviourist theories of learning) and the rise of communicative language teaching.
The separate guide to Communicative Language Teaching takes it on from there.
You should go to those guides (which open in new tabs) for more on the background and the story so far.

We have a whole guide on the Natural Approach, not because it is hugely influential in itself, but because the theories (or, better, hypotheses) which underlie it have been demonstrably influential in themselves.
The approach takes its name from the book by Krashen and Terrell (1983) which outlined an approach to language teaching based on a set of fundamental hypotheses.  It is these we need to focus on before we can discuss methodology arising from them.


5

5 key hypotheses

In 1982, Krashen published Second Language Acquisition and Second Language Learning in which he set out five hypotheses.  What follows here is a very brief summary.
The full text is, fortunately, now available at http://www.sdkrashen.com/content/books/sl_acquisition_and_learning.pdf.  Please look there for the background, evidence and more detail.

Hypothesis 1: THE ACQUISITION/LEARNING DISTINCTION
shoes
Krashen distinguishes between learning (a conscious process) and acquisition (an almost unconscious process).
Acquisition is a process similar to the way in which children acquire their first language(s).  It requires meaningful and frequent interaction in the language in which the speakers are not focused on form but on meaning.
Learning is, in contrast, a formal procedure which focuses on the explanation of rules and correction of language form.
It should be noted that Krashen was not explicitly stating that one is better than the other.  Indeed, he goes to some lengths to state that

the classroom can be of value, and in fact generally is of value, in language acquisition as well as in language learning
Krashen, 2002:48 (emphasis in the original)

What he is saying is that informal acquisition, given sufficient and intensive exposure is also an efficient way of acquiring a foreign language.
Many would agree, drawing on personal experience, that simply picking up a language in informal but frequent and intensive settings is an efficient and effective way of learning a foreign language and many would also agree, having unsuccessfully tried to learn a foreign language in a classroom, that formal learning settings are not invariably successful.
There are also those who profoundly disagree and aver that language learning is a conscious process.  See the guide to noticing (linked below) for a little more on this and some other ways of describing the learning process.
Hypothesis 2: THE MONITOR HYPOTHESIS
monitor
Following on from the primary distinction, Krashen posits that learners actively monitor their output in a foreign language.  To do this they use their learned rather than acquired knowledge.  An utterance is initiated and the learned system is used to monitor its accuracy.
The key point is that monitoring can, of course, only follow production although some have suggested that some learners delay producing language until they have mentally rehearsed what to say and monitored it internally.  This produces hesitant, slow production.  They are over-monitoring.
On the other hand, there are learners who under-monitor, producing immediate but wildly inaccurate language.
In writing, the same considerations apply but monitoring can be an ongoing process because of the nature of the skill.
Hypothesis 3: THE NATURAL ORDER HYPOTHESIS
order
With back-up from a body of research and a deal of number crunching in the book, Krashen asserts that grammatical language morphemes in particular are acquired in a certain order.  That order for English is:
order
(Krashen and Terrell (1983; 29))
So, for example, learners will acquire in this order utterances such as:
    She coming + cat → cats + She is happy
    She is coming + the cat / a cat
    She came
    She arrived + She arrives
    Mary's cat
Moreover, the hypothesis claims that any attempt to cheat the system by teaching items out of order will be fruitless.
Hypothesis 4: THE INPUT HYPOTHESIS
transfer
The argument here is that, for optimum effect, the input a learner receives should be a) comprehensible and b) just above the level of the learner.  This is sometimes abbreviated to INPUT + 1 or just i + 1.
Many see this as an intuitively correct hypothesis for if the input is incomprehensible, no sense can be made of it and no learning can take place but if the input is below the learners' competence, they are not challenged to improve, given the opportunity to acquire new language or notice the gap between his / her own production and the heard / read models.
Hypothesis 5: THE AFFECTIVE FILTER HYPOTHESIS
coffee
This suggests that high levels of, e.g., stress, or low levels of, e.g., self-esteem will act as inhibitors of the learning-acquisition process.  Low levels of stress and high levels of self-esteem and motivation will have the reverse effect.

Naturally, such a broad and significant range of hypotheses has not been without its critics.  However, whether one accepts the truth of all the hypotheses or not, there are clearly classroom implications to consider.  Consider how you would fill in the right-hand column in this table and then click on it for some comments.

natural approach task


action

The Natural Approach in action

To give you a flavour of how teaching and learning are carried out following the Natural Approach, here are some statements from a handbook of the approach (Krashen and Terrell, 1983).

  1. Your teacher will speak French exclusively.  You may answer in either English or French.  You are free to use English until you yourself feel ready to try speaking French.  You should not try to use French until you are comfortable doing so.
  2. When you do try to speak in the new language, the teacher is interested in what you have to say – not whether you have said it perfectly.  Neither you nor the teacher will be overly concerned with grammar errors in your speech while you are a beginner.
  3. You do not have to use full sentences.  You may talk in short phrases or even use just one word when that is appropriate.
  4. Remember that as long as you understand what the teacher is saying you are acquiring French.  This means that you should focus on what is being said, the message, rather than how it is being said.

The first of these is known as delayed oral practice and has received quite a lot of attention.  In the past, the assumption has been that intensive oral practice from the outset of the learning process will be effective.  This is now not taken for granted and some evidence shows that learners, especially beginners, need a period of time to assimilate what they hear before being asked to produce the target language.  In other words, to build competence before performance.

Many have noted that this kind of approach is not very far removed from an 'ordinary' communicative-approach classroom.


sceptic

Criticisms of the Natural Approach and Krashen's theories

Hypothesis 1: THE ACQUISITION/LEARNING DISTINCTION
There is some implication here that acquisition is in some way better than learning whereas the two are complementary.  Krashen has been quoted as taking the stance that the study of form actually has no place in the language-learning classroom.  As he and Terrell state the case:
The mistake the innovators have made is to assume that a conscious understanding of grammar is a prerequisite to acquiring communicative competence.  That such an understanding might be helpful in some situations for some students is not in question – that it is a prerequisite for all students is patently false. (op cit., p16).
Others have argued just the opposite:
... language learning is essentially learning how grammar functions in the achievement of meaning and it is a mistake to suppose otherwise. .... A communicative approach does not involve the rejection of grammar.  On the contrary, it involves a recognition of its central mediating role in the use of and learning of language. (Widdowson, 1990: 97/8)
Hypothesis 2: THE MONITOR HYPOTHESIS
The criticism here is that the existence or function of the Monitor has never been explicitly demonstrated.  The fact that the Monitor can, by definition, only function after an utterance has been produced seems to imply that it cannot slow down production.  It is also, of course, impossible, or at least very hard, to determine whether a learner is consciously or unconsciously applying a rule.
Hypothesis 3: THE NATURAL ORDER HYPOTHESIS
There's a built-in contradiction here.  Having first asserted that language learning (or acquisition) is not about grammar, Krashen is now proposing that a grammatical, structural syllabus should be followed.  How this can be reconciled with a natural, exposure-rich acquisition programme is not clear.
Secondly, the situation is more complex than it is painted.  The order may be different in production and comprehension, variable across languages, variable depending on the structure of the learner's first language and so on.  Worse, the acquisition of, say, the copular verb in He is a teacher may not be the same as the acquisition of the verb in He is idiotic.
A further issue here is that the research has focused on a limited range of learners (mostly Spanish speakers) and on a limited range of structures (mostly simple morphemes) so cannot include evidence from learners whose first languages are more distant from English in terms of grammar and lexis and or concerning more complex structures such as case endings, causatives, passive-voice constructions and much more.
Hypothesis 4: THE INPUT HYPOTHESIS
First of all, it is very nearly impossible to determine exactly where INPUT + 1 would apply to all learners in all situations and hard to determine 'level' at all in some cases.  Others have pointed out that Krashen nowhere explains how this works or how it aids learning.  It has also been pointed out that this is, in any case, what experienced teachers do all the time.  They alter their own production to exactly the point where they are understood but challenging.  They also select materials in which some of the input may be beyond i+1 and some below precisely in order to encourage their learners to notice aspects of the language.  It is arguable that there are many more factors at work here.
Hypothesis 5: THE AFFECTIVE FILTER HYPOTHESIS
The argument here is that it is clear that emotional and other factors play a part in language learning but that there is no need to hypothesise a 'filter' to explain the fact.  Furthermore, of course, one learner's added stress factor is another's motivating challenge.  One could argue, for example, that having the entire lesson content translated into the learners' first language would reduce their stress but that would fly in the face of the learning / acquisition hypothesis and much else.
Furthermore, much of Krashen's work in this area rests on studies of adolescents and it is unclear how applicable it is to adults or younger children.


Related guides
the history and development of ELT for more background
Communicative Language Teaching for a guide to the general area of CLT
noticing for more on a key hypothesis
input for more on how input may become output


There's a short, easy test on this.


References:
Krashen, S, 2002, Second Language Acquisition and Second Language Learning, First internet edition, retrieved from http://www.sdkrashen.com/content/books/sl_acquisition_and_learning.pdf
Krashen, SD & Terrell, TD, 1983, The natural approach: Language acquisition in the classroom, London: Prentice Hall Europe
Widdowson, H, 1990, Aspects of Language Teaching, Oxford: Oxford University Press